ATEG Archives

December 2008

ATEG@LISTSERV.MIAMIOH.EDU

Options: Use Monospaced Font
Show Text Part by Default
Show All Mail Headers

Message: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]
Topic: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]
Author: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]

Print Reply
Subject:
From:
Craig Hancock <[log in to unmask]>
Reply To:
Assembly for the Teaching of English Grammar <[log in to unmask]>
Date:
Mon, 8 Dec 2008 13:31:26 -0500
Content-Type:
text/plain
Parts/Attachments:
text/plain (355 lines)
Just one correction. The construct should read by X (participle head), Y 
(noun phrase) Z's or Z'd (finite verb phrase.)

Craig

Craig Hancock wrote:
> Jim,
>   Bob just pointed out that my reply to him was off list. I'm copying 
> it in here, along with the post to me that I was responding to 
> (below.)  I hope that makes sense.
> Craig
> Bob,
>   Like you, I'm sure, I wouldn't respond to the sentence without 
> looking at its context in the paper and without some sense of the 
> student. Was it just a slip on their part? The two really's also don't 
> work for me. But if it is worth paying attention to, it is worth 
> playing with. The student owns the sentence.
>  That aside, it seems to me to half way follow a common construct:  By 
> X (participle head), X (noun phrase) Y's (finite verb).
>  By sleeping in class, you missed half the lecture.
>  By getting angry, Charlie lost all chance for the job.
> Our expectations depend on the familiarity of the construct, not on 
> some innate grammar that predated our interactions with the world. 
> Since it's not an entirely fixed construction (it has variable slots), 
> construction grammar would call it schematic.
>  If the student seems to be comfortable with prescriptive grammar, I 
> might point out that a "by" phrase isn't supposed to act as subject. 
> But that is a different frame of reference.
>   If the grammar is innate, shouldn't the student know it already? If 
> it's not, then it helps to have someone mentor the student along. 
> Either they are already comfortable with the construction (and just 
> lapsed in attention), or we can take our time to model it out.
>  Learning a language requires "a prodigious amount of learning", and 
> this is one example.
>
> Craig
>  
> Robert Yates wrote:
> Craig,
>
> I'm not dismissing your alternative view out of hand.  I'm trying to
> figure out how it applies to REAL problems I confront as a writing
> teacher.  I don't understand how this view provides any insights into
> what my students do, and more importantly, what I do.
>
> I provided you with a REAL example of a mixed construction from a REAL
> student text. (1)  By taking time out of your day to get something for 
> someone else
> just really shows that you really care about them.
>
> Why don't you want to share with the listserv how your perspective
> accounts for such a sentence?
>
> Because you haven't done that yet, I will try to figure out what it
> means.  Consider the Langacker quote as a way to account for sentence
> (1).
>
> “The thrust of the content requirement is that the linguistic knowledge
> we ascribe to speakers should be limited to elements of form and meaning
> found in actually occurring expressions, or which derive from such
> elements via the basic psychological phenomena listed in 1.31:
> association, automatization, schematization, and categorization. By
> keeping our feet on the ground, this restriction assures both
> naturalness and theoretical austerity.”
> It seems to me that Langacker is saying the writer of (1) must have
> encountered such a construction in other contexts.  Is that correct? 
> The obvious implication is that we as teachers much find out what those
> contexts are and figure out ways for students to ignore such examples. 
> Is that correct?
>
> Of course, as teachers, how do WE know there is something inappropriate
> with (1) if "the linguistic knowledge we ascribe to speakers [is]
> limited to elements of form and meaning found in actually occurring
> expressions"?  I know I don't read texts that contain mixed
> constructions, except for my own student texts.  So, where did my
> knowledge come from that these structures that I have only encountered
> in student writing are inappropriate if my knowledge is based on
> actually occurring expressions?
>
> Craig, you want teachers on the list to take an alternative theory of
> language that is based on actual language we are exposed to.  From a
> teaching perspective, I'm trying to do that and I don't like the answer
> I come up with for students and the kinds of "innovative" sentences they
> write and my own judgments about those sentences. I must be wrong 
> because you are an experienced writing teacher and you
> find the perspective useful.  Please explain why it is useful for you.
>
> Bob Yates, University of Central Missouri
>
>
>
> Craig Hancock wrote:
>> Jim,
>>
>>   That's a very polite and thoughtful post. I have responded to Bob's 
>> post (after you sent this), and I hope it meets your objections. I 
>> would be happy to clarify as best I can if you still have questions. 
>> I'd like to say, also, that I don't want to pretend to be a principal 
>> spokesperson for cognitive grammar. I have found it very interesting 
>> and am trying to pass on a developing understanding. A delay in 
>> responding may just be my concern with representing views that are 
>> not only my own.
>>   My original post was aimed more at the shape actual explorations of 
>> language might take in a public school curriculum. I wasn't aiming at 
>> responding to error, but am happy to include it.
>>
>> Craig
>>  
>> Kenkel, Jim wrote:
>>>       I read this list but don't often post to it. However, this 
>>> last contribution from Craig compels me to respond.   Craig's 
>>> response to Bob Yates's post seems to do two things: 1) it repeats 
>>> the claims that prompted Bob's question in the first place about how 
>>> the theoretical  claims of Langacker and Biber provide teachers with 
>>> insight into the language use of student writers; 2) it seems to 
>>> seek to marginalize Bob's contribution to the discussion as being  
>>> socially innappropriate.  What it doesn't do is respond to Bob's 
>>> carefully and clearly posed question, the answer/s to which would be 
>>> certainly relevant to any teacher who has looked at his or her 
>>> students' writing from a language perspective.
>>>
>>>    It doesn't serve the list to characterize Bob's post as hostile.  
>>> I suspect that anyone who asks clear questions and receives no 
>>> answer to them would feel some frustration. Given Craig's prominence 
>>> in discussions on this list, I was interested to see his response 
>>> but was disappointed in the lack of response to the content of the 
>>> post.  I am interested in how the list generally might respond to 
>>> Bob's question because the answers might prompt me to do more 
>>> reading in cognitive grammar and usage-based grammar to learn what 
>>> insights they might offer me as a language teacher and as a writing 
>>> teacher. At this point, given my interests, I am no closer to 
>>> knowing how they might help me than I was before.
>>>
>>>     Given the complexity of the concepts of language, language use, 
>>> and writing, it is very safe to assume that no one perspective can 
>>> answer all the questions we have. I don't read Bob's post(s) as 
>>> marginalizing any perspective. He has only asked if cognitive and 
>>> usage-based grammars can help with issues of  recognizing and 
>>> responding to "error" in student writing. So far, the question has 
>>> not received a response.
>>>
>>>           Jim Kenkel, Eastern Kentucky University
>>> ________________________________________
>>> From: Assembly for the Teaching of English Grammar 
>>> [[log in to unmask]] On Behalf Of Craig Hancock 
>>> [[log in to unmask]]
>>> Sent: Monday, December 08, 2008 10:08 AM
>>> To: [log in to unmask]
>>> Subject: Re: Quick note on education and linguistic theory (was RE: 
>>> Correct)
>>>
>>>  Bob,
>>>     I’m not sure why you react with so much hostility to an attempt 
>>> to present an alternative point-of-view. You seem more interested in 
>>> debunking it than you are in learning about it; perhaps I’m wrong. 
>>> Other people on list may in fact be more interested in it than you 
>>> are. And I’m not sure why you would characterize it as “Craig’s 
>>> position” when I’m quoting others or simply assume you know my 
>>> position when you have been exposed to only a small part of it.
>>>    What I said, that you reacted to as a statement against 
>>> intuition, is the following:
>>>
>>>
>>> Among other things, cognitive linguists don't find it particularly
>>> useful to look at manufactured sentences like "*Mary is someone that
>>> people like her as soon as they see" and then ask why they don't seem
>>> grammatical. They find it more productive to look at the sentences that
>>> actually occur.
>>>     I didn’t say that we don’t have intuitions about language or 
>>> that intuitions aren’t important. In a usage based system, the 
>>> belief is that these grow out of use.
>>>
>>>      Langacker calls the above constraint The Content Requirement:
>>>      “The thrust of the content requirement is that the linguistic 
>>> knowledge we ascribe to speakers should be limited to elements of 
>>> form and meaning found in actually occurring expressions, or which 
>>> derive from such elements via the basic psychological phenomena 
>>> listed in 1.31: association, automatization, schematization, and 
>>> categorization. By keeping our feet on the ground, this restriction 
>>> assures both naturalness and theoretical austerity.” (Cognitive 
>>> Grammar: a basic introduction p. 25).
>>>
>>>    Here’s a quote from Biber, from the same anthology (Kemmer and 
>>> Barlow) I cited yesterday.
>>>     “Studies of use are concerned with actual practice, and the 
>>> extent to which linguistic patterns are common or rare, rather than 
>>> focusing exclusively on potential grammaticality. As such, adequate 
>>> investigations of language use must be empirical, analyzing the 
>>> functions and distribution of language features in natural discourse 
>>> contexts.”
>>>
>>>    Here he is again (et. Al.) in The Longman Student Grammar:
>>> “Traditionally, both in theory and in pedagogical practice, grammar 
>>> has been separate from vocabulary, as if they were two totally 
>>> independent aspects of language and language learning. This 
>>> separation is artificial, as becomes evident to anyone who uses a 
>>> large corpus for studying grammar. What becomes clear is that, when 
>>> they use a language, people bring together their knowledge of word 
>>> behavior (lexis) with their knowledge of grammatical patterns. These 
>>> two aspects of language interact in lexico-grammatical patterns.”
>>>
>>>      These are not trivial perspectives, and I don’t think it serves 
>>> the list to try to dismiss them summarily.
>>>
>>> Craig
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> Robert Yates wrote:
>>>
>>> This is a list about the role of grammar in the classroom.  Whatever 
>>> we mean by grammar must be grounded in some theory of language.  
>>> Therefore, there is something fundamentally wrong in the following 
>>> formulation.
>>>
>>> Craig writes:
>>>
>>> I don't think it is useful to the list to have an argument for
>>> different approaches, especially since a more articulate presentation
>>> of these views is available within the literature.
>>>
>>> ****
>>> Let's consider what Craig says is a view of language that must be 
>>> taken seriously.
>>> Craig quotes Kemmer and Barlow:
>>>
>>>    "Because the linguistic system is so closely tied to usage, it 
>>> follows
>>> that theories of language should be grounded in an observation of data
>>> from actual uses of language....Intuitions about constructed data
>>> cannot be treated as the sole, or even primary, source of evidence as
>>> to the nature and properties of the linguistic system." (Kemmer and
>>> Barlow, from the introduction to the same text.)
>>> ***
>>>
>>> Even corpus linguists have to use intuitions to decide what relevant 
>>> examples are from their corpus.
>>>
>>> [An example from Biber et al.'s Grammar of Spoken and Written 
>>> Language, a corpus based grammar of English.
>>>
>>> The identifying pattern
>>> Clauses following the identifying pattern answer the question 'Which 
>>> one is/was X?'  The copular verb is invariably be.  . . .
>>>
>>> My headmistress was the president of the Shakespeare league. 
>>> (conversation)
>>> The only reliable source of work is the water industry. 
>>> (newspaper)   (page 146)
>>> **
>>> My observation: Only intuitions about those example sentences allow 
>>> Biber et al. to say such a pattern answers the question.  NOTHING in 
>>> actual sentences says they answer such questions.  On almost every 
>>> page in Biber et al. are descriptions of the structures that are 
>>> based strictly on intuitions.]
>>>
>>> Let's take seriously the notion that "constructed data cannot be 
>>> treated as the sole, or even primary, source of evidence as to the 
>>> nature and properties of the linguistic system" FOR PEDAGOGICAL 
>>> PURPOSES.
>>>
>>> Consider the sentence from a real essay a student wrote.
>>>
>>> (1)  By taking time out of your day to get something for someone 
>>> else just really shows that you really care about them.
>>>
>>> If the source of knowledge about the language system is from actual 
>>> language use, what language sources was the writer of sentence (1) 
>>> exposed to for her to produce such a sentence?  I sure would like to 
>>> know how an approach to language which claims our knowledge of 
>>> language comes from "real language" answers that question.
>>>
>>> More importantly, as writing teachers, how do we KNOW that sentence 
>>> (1) is problematic.  What kinds of language were WE exposed to that 
>>> accounts for our judgment about sentence (1)?  If we have never been 
>>> exposed to mixed constructions and were never explicitly taught they 
>>> are problematic (as writing teachers, were we?), how do we recognize 
>>> them?  Under the approach Craig says we should consider, our 
>>> intuitions are based on the language we have been exposed to.
>>>
>>> As teachers of grammar and writing, we encounter strings written by 
>>> our students that are not in the texts they read.  And, just as 
>>> importantly, those strings our students write are not in the texts 
>>> WE read.  Yet, we are able to make judgments about those strings all 
>>> the time.   If usage is so fundamental to our knowledge of language, 
>>> what is the nature of the language we are exposed to that accounts 
>>> for our judgments.  (Does anyone regularly note that sentences like 
>>> (1) don't occur in writing?  How do you note the absence of 
>>> something if your only knowledge is based on what you are exposed to?)
>>>
>>> Of course, it is always possible that we possess no innate knowledge 
>>> about language, as Herb points out.  And, it possible that there is 
>>> no competence/performance distinction.  However, Jim Kenkel and I 
>>> have proposed, assuming innateness and difference between competence 
>>> and performance, that some of the "innovative" structures student 
>>> write, like sentence (1), can be explained.
>>>
>>> A theory of language is fundamental for what we as teachers of 
>>> grammar and writing do.  What Craig is proposing as a theory of 
>>> language can't explain what our students do and, more importantly, 
>>> what we as their teachers do when we respond to their writing.
>>>
>>> Bob Yates, University of Central Missouri.
>>>
>>> To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
>>> interface at:
>>>      http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
>>> and select "Join or leave the list"
>>>
>>> Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
>>> interface at: http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html and 
>>> select "Join or leave the list"
>>>
>>> Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/
>>>
>>> To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
>>> interface at:
>>>      http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
>>> and select "Join or leave the list"
>>>
>>> Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/
>>>
>>>
>>>   
>>
>> To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
>> interface at:
>>     http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
>> and select "Join or leave the list"
>>
>> Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/
>>
>>
>
> To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
> interface at:
>     http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
> and select "Join or leave the list"
>
> Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/
>
>

To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web interface at:
     http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
and select "Join or leave the list"

Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/

ATOM RSS1 RSS2