ATEG Archives

March 2006

ATEG@LISTSERV.MIAMIOH.EDU

Options: Use Monospaced Font
Show Text Part by Default
Show All Mail Headers

Message: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]
Topic: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]
Author: [<< First] [< Prev] [Next >] [Last >>]

Print Reply
Subject:
From:
"Stahlke, Herbert F.W." <[log in to unmask]>
Reply To:
Assembly for the Teaching of English Grammar <[log in to unmask]>
Date:
Wed, 15 Mar 2006 09:06:12 -0500
Content-Type:
text/plain
Parts/Attachments:
text/plain (235 lines)
Eduard,

A tree diagram is constrained to show the structure of the sentence with
the word order the speaker/writer has used.  If one works
derivationally, then word order can change, but movement rules of the
sort that were used in 60s and early 70s generative grammar were far too
powerful to be of much theoretical use and had to be constrained in a
variety of ways.  I don't know how a GB, P&P, Minimalist, or later
grammarian would handle the problem because I don't do that sort of
theory any more.  One of the differences between a tree diagram, by
which I mean a phrase structure tree, and a Reed-Kellogg diagram is that
the RK will shift word order in some cases so that constituents are in a
more canonical order.  Tree diagrams don't do this.

But you may be referring to a tree-drawing convention that I'm not
familiar with.

Herb


Dear Herb:

The sentence "Running from the back of his skull down to the front is 
a patch of white hair that opens up into his lip" is an example a 
sentence where syntactic means are employed for expressing focus and 
emphasis. The expression of focus and emphasis is achieved in this 
sentence through *marked word order,* that is, "the movement of a 
constituent into a position in the sentence where we whould not 
ordinarily expect to find it." 

Specifically, as Johanna stated, this is a case of "preposing" 
or "fronting," of a *present participle,* as in the example:

"Sitting at the kitchen table was our missing uncle."

In our case, the preposed present participle is *running from the 
back of his skull down to the front.* 


The question is if this case of preposing can be treated in a similar 
way to the WH-fronting, that is, in a double tree structure which 
would indicate a subject-operator inversion shown in a tree movement. 
I searched, but I haven't found any information on this matter 
concerning "present participle fronting" in the textbooks I searched. 

My personal perspective is that because the propositional meaning of 
the sentence does not change with the preposing the tree does not 
need to show movement, but needs to follow the basic English sentence 
order, SVO. A tree diagram, should therefore begin with the the noun 
phrase which functions as a subject and continue with the predicate, 
all subcomponents falling under the major parts according to their 
functions in the sentence. 

I would be interested to know what Johanna thinks about this matter.

Eduard 


On Tue, 14 Mar 2006, Herbert F.W. Stahlke wrote...

>Here's another post from Johanna that's well worth reading.  As I 
told =
>her, I pretty much gave up on theoretical syntax back in the mid 70s 
=
>when I couldn't wrap existing theory around the serial verb =
>constructions I was working on in West African languages at the 
time.  =
>It's hard within any version of MIT-rooted syntactic theory to deal 
with =
>sentences that have multiple verbs in a single clause with no =
>coordination or complementation involved.
>
>Herb
>
>
>-----Original Message-----
>From: Johanna Rubba [mailto:[log in to unmask]]
>Sent: Tue 3/14/2006 3:34 PM
>To: Stahlke, Herbert F.W.
>Cc: Assembly for the Teaching of English Grammar
>Subject: Re: What Is This? Herb's Analysis
>=20
>Herb,
>
>Again I'd ask you to post this.
>
>As to the matter of existential sentences, an alternative name for 
them=20
>is _presentative_ sentences, since their purpose is to "present" 
the=20
>existence of the subject as news. This avoids the ambiguity of 
using=20
>"existential" to name two different kinds of sentence.
>
>I think I have a good proof for the idea that the sentence in 
question=20
>is presentative. I tried the tag-question test to find the subject 
of=20
>the sentence, but I got a presentative result:
>
>1. Running from the back of his skull down to the front is a patch 
of=20
>white hair that opens up into his lips, isnt there?   vs.
>2.  Running from the back of his skull down to the front is a patch 
of=20
>white hair that opens up into his lips, isn't it?
>
>Although both sound bad (it's often hard to make tags for 
questions=20
>with initial phrases other than the subject), I think the first 
sounds=20
>much better, relatively speaking. Creating presentative sentences 
is=20
>one of the major uses of "there." Such sentences have "there" in 
the=20
>tag:
>
>3. There's a fly in your soup, isn't there?
>
>Non-presentative sentences put a pronoun in the tag that agrees 
with=20
>the subject:
>
>4. The girl is in the school band, isn't she?
>
>It is also clear that, even though 2 is awful, we would still 
interpret=20
>the "it" as having "a patch of white hair (etc.)" as antecedent.
>
>Going over this discussion in my mind, along with past ones that 
have=20
>gone on a bit, with different analyses of a sentence, I can 
understand=20
>why a lot of list subscribers find such discussions more 
mystifying=20
>than helpful. There are unambiguously correct syntactic analyses 
of=20
>many sentences, esp. if you adhere to the most conventional 
definitions=20
>of terms among linguists (Ed Vavra, please don't start up again 
about=20
>the definition of "main clause"). There is only one correct 
syntactic=20
>analysis for the sentence in question. "Running from the back of 
his=20
>skull down to the front" is a preposed subject complement 
(predicate=20
>adjective in traditional terms); "a patch of white hair that opens 
up=20
>into his lips" is the subject. It would be nice if subscribers who 
have=20
>posted other syntactic analyses would acknowledge this. If they 
are=20
>using definitions and interpretations that are not in common use 
among=20
>linguists, this should be made clear; they can still prefer their 
own=20
>analysis, but at least other subscribers would understand why and 
how=20
>there can be disagreement. I think I always make a point of saying=20
>whether my analyses come from Cognitive Grammar, for instance.
>
>As to drawing a tree for this sentence, you would have to start 
with=20
>the version of the sentence that does not prepose the "running" 
phrase:
>
>5. A patch of white hair that opens up into his lips is running 
from=20
>the back of his skull down to the front.
>
>Then, if one does this with a tree at all, another tree is needed 
for=20
>the "running"-initial version, and that tree would have to show 
the=20
>"movement" (I don't believe in movement, myself). Ignoring updated=20
>versions of generative grammar that include things like a C-node 
and=20
>INFL (or whatever the current practice is), an old-style tree 
would=20
>reflect the following phrase structure rules for the non-preposing=20
>sentence:
>
>S -> NP   VP
>NP =3D A patch of white hair that opens up into his lips
>VP =3D is running from the back of his skull down to the front
>
>VP ->  V   AP
>V =3D is
>AP =3D running from the back of his skull down to the front
>
>The old style of tree diagramming did not use trees to show 
transformed=20
>structures. At that time, there was no X-bar syntax, no CP or C 
nodes,=20
>and no IP nodes. Even if we have those at hand, I don't think it 
would=20
>be correct to put the "running" phrase under the C node. You would 
also=20
>have to get the subject into the IP, which seems incoherent. Maybe=20
>someone who is better-versed in the current generative theory can 
tell=20
>us how (and whether) one can draw a tree for the preposed 
sentence.=20
>It's important to realize that (so far as I know) generative syntax 
has=20
>not abandoned the practice of starting with some kind 
of "underlying"=20
>structure and then operating on it. Maybe Optimality Theory doesn't 
do=20
>this, but I have not investigated Optimality as applied to syntax.
>
>Dr. Johanna Rubba, Associate Professor, Linguistics
>Linguistics Minor Advisor
>English Department
>California Polytechnic State University, San Luis Obispo
>E-mail: [log in to unmask]
>Tel.: 805.756.2184
>Dept. Ofc. Tel.: 805.756.2596
>Dept. Fax: 805.756.6374
>URL: http://www.cla.calpoly.edu/~jrubba
>
>To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web 
interface at:
>     http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
>and select "Join or leave the list"
>
>Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/

To join or leave this LISTSERV list, please visit the list's web interface at:
     http://listserv.muohio.edu/archives/ateg.html
and select "Join or leave the list"

Visit ATEG's web site at http://ateg.org/

ATOM RSS1 RSS2